The behaviourists have failed

Member ratings
  • Well argued: 61%
  • Interesting points: 71%
  • Agree with arguments: 54%
36 ratings - view all
The behaviourists have failed

Every day at 5pm is our national “Unhappy Hour”.  The brave and vigilant watch the Coronavirus Daily Update in fascination, horror, shock and scepticism. Next to a government Minister who will be “following the science” we may now find a senior Public Health behavioural scientist. Behavioural science is in the news.

For a variety of reasons many people want to understand, predict and change human behaviour: military strategists, advertising agencies, and political parties, to name a few. Whether or not the knowledge accumulated from a century of experiments on human behaviour is enough to qualify as a science is a moot point. Were it a science, you might expect behavioural scientists to have become millionaires from buying and selling shares, playing poker, or profiling serial killers.

But they aren’t rich and they don’t identify killers, except in the movies. In reality the human sciences find it difficult to produce hard metrics that predict behaviour. They have not achieved, for example, anything equivalent to measuring the length of the Coronavirus’s RNA — it’s much longer than that of most viruses — or plotting a space-craft’s trajectory to Saturn. Instead many of us are left with the impression that generalities about human behaviour can be generally unhelpful.

Individual human beings are different. We all recognise that. And we also know that individuals behave differently to groups. People share different cultures which may weight different characteristics and values differently, influencing how individuals act. And groups of human beings present different characteristics from each other, some of which can be important, not least susceptibility to different maladies, sickle-cell anaemia or diabetes for example, or living in different sized family groups. The differences are shockingly illustrated by the high death rates from Coronavirus amongst British BAME people. Disentangling the causes will be complex.

The biggest established group difference in mortality from Coronavirus is that between rich and poor, which means “morbidity” — the rate of disease in a particular community or population — has as much to do with political choices as individual behaviour. This fact, well known and acknowledged in Public Health, has become disturbingly clear to more people thanks to quantitative reporting and analysis of deaths and infections during the pandemic.

Predictions about the reaction of the British public when asked to stay at home and socially distance were incorrect: this misreading seems to have resulted initially in an assumption that the period of lockdown had to be minimal or people would revolt. Consequently lockdown was disastrously delayed and infection built up. This is not the wisdom of hindsight. In early March, when Northern Italy was already in serious trouble, how could the Prime Minister have been unaware of the gravity of the situation and the potential exponential spread of the virus?

Was the government fearful of the implications of people not “following the science” or were they simply choosing from among a range of policy responses provided by behavioural scientists? Was the government too pre-occupied with the economic consequences? The subsequent rules, to stay at home, for weeks, legally enforceable by the police, were hardly an example of “nudging” — that is, using material or social rewards or mild forfeits, to get people to do the right thing. It was all very mysterious, which is just the way government, unchallenged, likes it.

Government health policies are not decided in a vacuum. Reducing smoking took years of persuasion, public education, support for quitters, restrictions on marketing and then legislation, all in the face of opposition from the tobacco industry and smokers. Similarly, tackling the obesity epidemic, reducing alcohol and sugar consumption, was and is resisted by corporate interests. Up against such odds — and business is already lobbying for an end to lockdown — the smartest of the behavioural scientists faces a daunting task. Meanwhile government can, and does, finesse the problem of the pressure on the NHS by blaming all those who inconveniently live into their 80s and acquire “co-morbidities”.

Now the benighted behavioural scientists are called on to advise about messaging in preparation for the un-lockdown. This means finding a way to get the people who are not designated key workers and not in fear of destitution, and who have been encouraged to return to work, to choose to do so. But how can the government calm public fears and convey messages to different groups while at the same time avoiding confusion? How can behaviour be changed without compulsion? On this occasion we can expect some nugatory nudging; fear of death has concentrated the mind and the British public will decide for itself.

Understanding, predicting and changing public behaviour in response to the threat of coronavirus infection is now at the centre of the political stage. But behaviour is not hard science; it’s often a matter of well or ill-informed opinion — and opinions conflict.

Member ratings
  • Well argued: 61%
  • Interesting points: 71%
  • Agree with arguments: 54%
36 ratings - view all

You may also like